Currently viewing a development environment

Even vampire bats do social distancing when their friends are sick

New research finds that bats care for their ill family members, but don't socialize as much with their sick friends

Olivia Box

Natural Resources and Forest Ecology

University of Vermont

A common way illnesses move is through physical interaction – touching hands or hugging, having close conversations, and being coughed or sneezed on are all common ways that pathogens are spread. But the intricacies of how sickness spreads are of increasing importance as diseases like COVID-19 spread around the world, prompting questions about what we really know about transmission.

“Epidemiological models have tried to predict how pathogens spread, assuming all interactions are the same," said Sebastian Stockmaier, a PhD student at the University of Texas. But this isn’t always the case.

New research, led by Stockmaier and published in the Journal of Animal Ecology from a group of researchers in the US and Panama explores how social behaviors in vampire bats were affected when the bats were sick. 

a researcher studying a small bat

Sebastian Stockmaier, the lead author, examining a vampire bat during the study

Photo courtesy of Sebastian Stockmaier

Vampire bats are highly social creatures, living in colonies of hundreds to thousands of individuals. Socialization is a key component of their survival strategy, as they care for one another through mutually beneficial behaviors such as reciprocal grooming and food sharing, the process of licking each other’s mouths to share food – the food being regurgitated blood. Relatives are most likely to food-share, and this behavior is more common between female bats than males

This behavior, while essential to survival, also plays a role in social structure. Studies have shown some bats will stay home while the rest go out hunting, waiting for them to return and food-share regurgitated blood. But, these lazy bats who take advantage of the food sharing process may eventually be treated like freeloaders and be denied food-sharing from bats who spend more time hunting. 

To measure how sickness impacts social behavior, both "sickness" and "social behavior" must be defined. Stockmaier and the other researchers defined "sickness" as an immune response to an infection, causing a bat to spend more energy fighting the infection than other activities. They wanted to examine how social behavior, including food-sharing and grooming, were affected when bats were sick.

The researchers monitored the social behavior of a small captive bat colony at the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute in Panama. Bats were injected with a bacteria that stimulated their immune systems and made them sick, so that they lacked energy for basic activities. Food (raw cattle blood) was dispensed through the trial, and the bats were free to interact with one another. To encourage food sharing, the researchers also periodically withheld food from some of the bats, keeping them isolated from the rest of the group for 26-28 hours. This simulated "missed foraging opportunities," or nights where the bats left their colony to feed but were unsuccessful. The researchers wanted to see if the bats with food would share with the fasting bats when they were returned to the colony.

Despite being sick, bats continued to socialize, but sickness changed how they did so. Sick bats groomed themselves less and received less grooming from healthy bats. The researchers attribute this to the symptoms of their illness itself, since the less active bats groomed other bats less, causing healthy bats to not return the favor.

Food-sharing is considered an essential habit for survival, and both sick and healthy bats continued to practice it. Bats that were injected with the bacteria (the "sick" bats) had been fasting before the injection, so they especially needed food. The sick bats then begged for food by licking another bat's mouth. Bats continued to beg and feed each other, despite being sick. The only notable behavioral change that Stockmaier and the other researchers observed was that sick bats groomed bats that were not closely related to them less often than they did when the "groomees" were healthy. 

Even in the midst of an epidemic, the vampire bats retained their familial structures – mothers continued to feed their offspring, regardless of who was sick. This shows that while sickness may make bats lethargic and less inclined to socialize, it doesn’t prevent them from doing so with close family members.

vampire bat snarling while sitting on wood log

Close-up of a vampire bat

Uwe Schmidt on Wikimedia Commons

Not all diseases are made equal. Many bats in North America are afflicted with white nose syndrome, a fungal disease with a high fatality rate that especially affects hibernating bats. A study on little brown bats found that white nose syndrome also interferes with bat socialization behaviors; sick bats were groomed less and were less likely to visit a water source, likely because the trip took a lot of energy.

Stockmaier compares the group's findings in their vampire bat experiment to a child who is sick at home: their parent will care for their child no matter what. Says Stockmaier, “If you think of it like social distancing – it’s not like you’re totally self-isolated. You’re probably with your family and still interacting in some way."

Comment Peer Commentary

We ask other scientists from our Consortium to respond to articles with commentary from their expert perspective.

Ankita Arora

Biochemistry

University of Colorado

This excellent piece talks and explores changes in social behavior patterns in bat upon being infected. I believe this strategy when improvised in future combined with animal swabs could be used to check spread of infection in animal. This might in turn prevent spilling of the virus from animals to humans as a host and prove to be useful to spread the infection in human too. There are efforts that screen bats, chicken and pig animal facilities to detect early spread of the virus that infect each of these species respectively.
It would be interesting to know if the scientists can differentiate between the two possibilities. Is lack of food sharing really symptoms of their illness i.e. returning the favor less when sick bats can’t groom or is it a self-care strategy healthy bats follow similar to like social distancing? Another interesting question that this brings up is that if healthy bats can really sense symptoms in sick bats? 

Olivia Box responds:

Hey Ankita! Some really interesting questions here. I spoke with the lead author about how different illnesses could effect bats, and this likely translates to other animals as well. How a virus or pathogen acts could illicit a different immune response. It would be really interesting to see this study replicated under different conditions. 

Matthew Vandermeulen

Biology

University at Buffalo

Nice article! One thing I am always curious about with studies done in captivity, is how well they reflect what occurs in the wild. For example, do they make it similarly difficult for the bats to find the food in captivity as it is in the wild? If not, I wonder if being fed in captivity makes them less conservative with food sharing because they learn that it will always become available. I also wonder if removing a bat from the other bats mimics the behavior of staying behind while the others go foraging. Maybe the bat being removed sends a different message, such as, ‘I would have came with you, but i was taken away and therefore deserve sympathy’. Have there been studies to track this behavior in the wild that helps to support this studies conclusions? One other question I have is this, do the bats try and prevent others that are sick from licking their face and begging for food? I wonder if they actually know that another bat is sick, or if healthy bats are sharing less with sick bats because sick bats can’t share with them. 

Olivia Box responds:

Hi Matthew! The study actually has been replicated in the wild and is currently under review-- stay tuned! Vampire bats live in large colonies (1,000-3,000 bats) and the research team is interested in how these interactions translate in a field setting.